DOI: 10.31038/CST.2020512
Abstract
Anger is one of the possible reactions to cancer. There are mutual influences between cancer and psychological status, with repercussions on the immune system. The aim of this study was to analyze differences in the experience, expression and control of anger by gender and to measure the relationship between anger, anxiety, depression, quality of life, and progression of the disease. We have conducted a cross-sectional study assessing 281 cancer patients, using the STAXI-2, HADS and a visual analogue scale to measure Quality of Life. Results: Females reported significantly higher State and Trait Anger scores and lower Anger Control Out scores than males. In the whole sample the anger subscale scores increased with levels of anxiety or depression. In males, high State, Trait and Expression Anger subscale scores resulted associated with low Quality of Life, among females, this relationship seems to be weak. No differences emerged on STAXI-2 scales and subscales between patients in progression of disease. Conclusions: Anxiety, depression and anger seem to be organized into a pattern of a general emotional reaction. Since immunotherapy is the anticancer treatment that increases the body’s natural defenses to fight disease, a balanced immune system has become the main concern. In conclusion, clinicians could gain important insights about their patients by looking at the result of validated self-report patient questionnaires, to identify patients with inadequate expression of emotion or too high levels of emotional reaction in order to improve quality of care and response to treatment.
Keywords
anger scale, cancer management, depression assessment, immunological characteristics quality of life
Introduction
Anger is an emotion that in these present times is making itself noticed in many contexts and in everyday life. It is the reaction to the frustration that people are experiencing due to the poor prospects of economic development, the lack of work, for young and old, etc. In oncology, clinicians are led to think that anger is the emotional reaction that characterizes the moments behind the diagnosis to then give way to emotional experiences of anxiety and then later depression. For a long time it was hypothesized that there could be a sequence of psychological reactions conditioned by the distance from the diagnosis. Today the experience and manifestations of anger must be studied in a more specific way, especially in light of the evidence that has shown a sex-specific differences in activity of emotion processing regions [1,2]. In medicine we know that sex hormones influence the onset and severity of various immuno-modulate disease [3,4], and the role of the psychological state on the disease and vice versa is known. Different diseases could be influenced by the psychological status and gender, just as diagnoses influence the psychological status of the patient, perhaps differently in the two sexes. If on the one hand the knowledge on prevalence of anxiety and depression is now consolidated [5], on the other, knowledge about the differences between the sexes in anger is still limited. Common sense leads us to think that the expression of anger outward is more frequent among males, although Aghaei [6] has found very similar anger scores between males and females in a sample of cancer patients admitted to the hospital for surgery. There is a need to study in depth the complex relationships between sex, psychological distress and disease, especially today that the use of immunotherapy treatments makes the picture more complicated. Scientific literature has proposed to distinguish among different components of anger. There are differences between expressing or inhibiting anger, between controlling it by adopting relaxation methods or rather strategies to shift attention towards other stimuli. Studies that investigated the aspects of suppression, repression, or restraint of anger [7] found that the suppressed anger was associated with health risk factors and early mortality for all-cause, included cardiovascular and cancer mortality, in women [6,8]. The inhibition of negative emotions plays an important role in how cancer patients adjust to the disease [9,10]. Low levels of anger were associated with maladaptive coping strategies to cancer [3,4]. Differently other researchers have found that a reaction of anger expression may be an important factor in the fight against cancer [11]. However, patients with high levels of intensity of anger should be evaluated carefully to determine whether the risk of acting out their anger represents a potential danger to themselves and others. Differently, moderate anger experiences may guide problem-solving behaviour and people may be more assertive than aggressive.
Anyways anger is a human emotion that can vary in component, intensity, expression and control [12] and each pattern can have different meaning and relationship with other emotional feeling and behavioural experiences for people. To study the multifaceted construct of anger the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2TM [13,14] has been developed and validated. The questionnaire has been developed to recognizes 11 components of experience, expression and control of anger. The State Anger refers to the intensity of the individual’s angry feelings at a specified time or at the time of testing. The State Anger splits into 3 domains: Feeling Angry, to Feel like Expressing Anger Verbally and Feel like Expressing Anger Physically. The first is related with annoyance, irritation, anger or fury. The second, with the desire to express anger by swearing, cursing, yelling or screaming. The third, reflects the inclination to hit someone or to break things. Trait anger measures a predisposition to become angry and is critical for understanding how often a person becomes angry. Anger experienced quickly and with little provocation reflects an Angry Temperament, as a predisposition, while the tendency to become angry when people receives negative feedback is Angry Reaction. Anger Expression Out and Anger Expression In describe the extent to which people expresses emotional experiences of anger in an outwardly negative and poorly controlled way or vice versa to be under-reactive, suppressing, repressing, or denying anger feelings because uncomfortable. Anger Control Out involves the expenditure of energy to monitor and control the expression of angry. People with high Anger Control Out may not be in touch with their emotions and may not recognize the need to act on solving the problem causing the anger. Last, Anger Control In reflects how a person attempts to relax and reduce angry before got out of control. The aim of this study was to measure the sexes differences among cancer patients related to: (1) experience, expression and control of anger; (2) relationship between anger components and anxiety, depression, quality of life, and progression of disease.
Materials and Methods
142 male cancer patients (mean age= 65.19; standard deviation, SD = 12.53) and 139 female cancer patients (mean age = 62.57; SD = 11.47) admitted for oncological treatment at the Day Hospital were consecutively enrolled in this study. Informed consent was obtained for all patients at the enrolment in the study. The Committee of Ethics of the hospital approved the study protocol (Prot. n. 39/CE/2017, 04 Oct 2017).
Questionnaires
State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2TM (STAXI-2) [13,14]. The 57-item STAXI-2 consists of six main scales (State Anger, Trait Anger, Anger Expression Out, Anger Expression In, Anger Control Out and Anger Control In) and five subscales (for State Anger: Feeling Angry, to Feel like Expressing Anger Verbally and Feel like Expressing Anger Physically; for Trait Anger: Angry Temperament and Angry Reaction) to measure experience, expression and control of anger. The State Anger scale assesses the intensity of anger as an emotional state at a data time. The Trait Anger scale measures how often angry feelings are experienced over time. The Anger Expression Out and Anger Expression In, Anger Control Out and Anger Control Out In scales assess four independent anger-related traits: expression of anger toward other persons or objects in the environment; holding in or suppressing angry feelings; controlling angry feelings by preventing the expression of anger toward other persons or objects in the environment; controlling suppressed angry feelings by calming down or cooling off, respectively. Each item has 4 options and the subjects graded themselves by using a 4-point Likert scale (from 1=almost never to 4=usually). In this study, internal consistency was good to excellent for all STAXI-2 scales, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranging from 0.69 (Anger Expression Out) to 0.93 (State Anger) for males, and from 0.72 (Anger Expression Out) to 0.93 (State Anger) for females.
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [15]. The HADS is a 14-item screening instrument for anxiety (7 items) and depression (7 items) in a non-psychiatric setting. Each item is scored from 0 (not present) to 3 (highly present). Separately for each scale (range 0–21), scores <8 suggest absence of disease, scores from 8 to 10 are considered borderline, and those >10 identify probable cases of anxiety or depression [16]. In this study, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were 0.78 and 0.79 for males and 0.82 and 0.75 for females, respectively for anxiety and depression.
Quality of life (QoL) and perception of severity and curability of the disease [17] were self- assessed by the patients using a 10-point rating scale (bad/excellent quality of life, very/not very severe, difficult/very easy to cure). Sociodemographic data and clinical information were also collected.
Statistical Analyses
For descriptive purpose we divided patients into two categories according to differences in education (<=13yrs of school versus >13yrs). Marital status was dichotomized into persons living alone (single, divorced, widowed) and into those cohabiting (married, cohabiting). Data about presence of metastasis, lymphectomia, and progression of disease were also dichotomized (absent versus present). Cancer type with less than 5 patients were grouped together and labeled “other”. For sub-analyses purpose, data about HADS anxiety and depression were divided into three groups (no disease, score 0–7; borderline, score 8–10, cases 11–21). All the raw questionnaire and 10-point rating scale scores were linearly transformed to 0–100 scales to allow comparability, using the
Formula:
Tscore = (X – Xmin)/X range)*n
where Tscore is the adjusted variable, X is the original variable, Xmin is the minimum observed value on the original variable and Xrange is the difference between the maximum potential score and the minimum potential score on the original variable and n is the upper limit of the rescaled variable. The Tscores are characterized by a distribution with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation (SD) of 10, with 0 and 100 assigned to the lowest and highest possible values, respectively. Continuous variables were reported as medians (and interquartile ranges [IQRs]) and as means (and SD or 95% Confidence Intervals, 95%CIs), and inferences were tested with parametric or non- parametric tests accordingly with their distribution. Even if the sample distribution of the values was asymmetric, the data were also presented as means to allow better visualization of the results, since the median often coincided with the quartiles. Categorical variables were reported as proportions and chi-squared or Fisher’s exact test were used for comparison. Cronbach’s alpha was adopted to evaluate internal consistency of questionnaires scales and subscales. The analyzes were conducted separately from gender. All statistical analyses have been performed using STATA, version 11.0 (StataCorp, College Station, Tex).
Results
A total of 281 cancer patients, 142 males and 139 females, aged from 31 to 88 years, were enrolled in the study. Sociodemographic and clinical features are reported in Table 1, separately for gender. Males were older at diagnosis (p=0.016), they had more frequently metastasis (p<0.001) and an advanced cancer stadium (p<0.001), a shorter length of disease (p=0.038) and less chemotherapeutic treatments (p=0.001), compared to females. In Table 2 mean values and 95%CIs of STAXI-2 and HADS domains, perception of qol, severity and curability of the disease are shown separately for males and females. Males reported lower mean scores in State (p=0.019) and Trait Anger (p=0.030), and higher mean scores in Anger Control In, than females. No differences emerged in Anger Expression, Out and In, and in Anger Control In between gender.
Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the sample, separately for gender.
Males |
Females |
|||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
pvalue* |
||
age, years (mean; 95%CI) |
65,19 |
63,11–67,27 |
62,57 |
60,65–64,49 |
0,069 |
|
education |
<13yrs |
113 |
79,58 |
105 |
75,54 |
0,417 |
>13yrs |
29 |
20,42 |
34 |
24,46 |
||
married |
no |
19 |
13,48 |
28 |
20,59 |
0,115 |
yes |
122 |
86,52 |
108 |
79,41 |
||
age at diagnosis (mean; 95%CI) |
63,13 |
60,96–65,30 |
59,43 |
57,34–61,52 |
0,016 |
|
length of disease, years |
2,06 |
1,41–2,70 |
3,17 |
2,33–4,00 |
0,038 |
|
0–1yrs |
98 |
69,01 |
79 |
56,83 |
||
2–8 yrs |
34 |
23,94 |
41 |
29,50 |
||
9+ yrs |
10 |
7,04 |
19 |
13,67 |
0.065 |
|
cancer type |
colon |
37 |
26,06 |
22 |
15,83 |
<0.001 |
melanoma |
26 |
18,31 |
24 |
17,27 |
||
lung |
34 |
23,94 |
14 |
10,07 |
||
breast |
0 |
0,00 |
46 |
33,09 |
||
vescica |
10 |
7,04 |
3 |
2,16 |
||
ovaio |
0 |
0,00 |
12 |
8,63 |
||
gastrico |
7 |
4,93 |
3 |
2,16 |
||
other |
28 |
19,72 |
15 |
10,79 |
||
cancer stadium |
I |
4 |
2,92 |
6 |
4,65 |
<0.001 |
II |
2 |
1,46 |
22 |
17,05 |
||
III |
26 |
18,98 |
23 |
17,83 |
||
IV |
105 |
76,64 |
78 |
60,47 |
||
linfectomia |
no |
67 |
47,18 |
33 |
23,74 |
<0.001 |
yes |
68 |
47,89 |
101 |
72,66 |
||
missing value |
7 |
4,93 |
5 |
3,60 |
||
metastasis at diagnosis |
no |
72 |
50,70 |
104 |
74,82 |
<0.001 |
yes |
69 |
48,59 |
35 |
25,18 |
||
missing value |
1 |
0,70 |
0 |
0,00 |
||
radiotherapy |
no |
111 |
78,17 |
91 |
65,47 |
<0.001 |
adjuvant |
5 |
3,52 |
33 |
23,74 |
||
palliative |
24 |
16,90 |
14 |
10,07 |
||
esclusiva |
2 |
1,41 |
1 |
0,72 |
||
CHT |
no |
103 |
72,54 |
63 |
45,32 |
<0.001 |
neoadjuvant |
4 |
2,82 |
3 |
2,16 |
||
adjuvant |
35 |
24,65 |
73 |
52,52 |
||
pre-CHT line (mean; 95%CI) |
2,30 |
1,70–2,91 |
4,03 |
3,33–4,72 |
<0.001 |
|
progression of disease |
no |
28 |
19,72 |
45 |
32,37 |
0,016 |
yes |
114 |
80,28 |
94 |
67,63 |
* Chi2 test; Student t test
Table 2: Descriptive statistics for STAXI-2 scales, HADS scales, patient perception of quality of life (QOL), cancer severity and curability.
males |
females |
||||
(N=142; 50,53%) |
(N=139; 49,47%) |
||||
mean |
95%CI |
mean |
95%CI |
p-value* |
|
STAXI |
|||||
Anger state |
7,84 |
5,47–10,21 |
11,45 |
8,71–14,18 |
0,019 |
feeling angry |
12,35 |
9,46–15,24 |
17,55 |
13,99–21,12 |
0,040 |
verbal anger |
7,56 |
4,50–10,62 |
12,09 |
8,46–15,72 |
0,023 |
physical anger |
3,62 |
1,56–5,67 |
4,70 |
2,61–6,69 |
0,472 |
Ange trait |
24,88 |
21,50–28,27 |
29,34 |
25,93–32,74 |
0,030 |
temperament |
19,07 |
15,59–22,56 |
25,00 |
21,61–28,39 |
0,002 |
reaction |
31,87 |
27,70–36,04 |
35,13 |
31,05–39,21 |
0,145 |
Anger expression |
|||||
out |
23,88 |
21,26–26,51 |
24,85 |
22,16–27,54 |
0,566 |
in |
34,33 |
31,12–37,54 |
33,69 |
30,10–37,29 |
0,577 |
Anger control |
|||||
out |
66,33 |
62,15–70,51 |
60,19 |
55,96–64,43 |
0,036 |
in |
58,51 |
54,56–62,46 |
57,27 |
53,23–61,28 |
0,723 |
HADS |
|||||
anxiety |
26,89 |
24,02–29,77 |
35,46 |
32,28–38,63 |
<0.001 |
depression |
27,20 |
24,16–30,23 |
31,41 |
28,35–34,48 |
0,054 |
QOL |
6,81 |
6,45–7,17 |
6,40 |
6,03–6,78 |
0,091 |
Cancer severity perception |
5,13 |
4,70–5,56 |
4,96 |
4,53–5,38 |
0,539 |
Cancer curability perception |
6,38 |
5,99–6,78 |
6,20 |
5,79–6,62 |
0,517 |
* Kruskal-wallis test
Males also reported lower mean score in HADS-Anxiety (p<0.001) and Depression scale (p=0.054). No significant differences emerged in perception of QoL, severity and curability of cancer between gender. Sixteen males (11.27%) and 32 females (23.02%) resulted probably-cases of anxiety (HADS- Anxiety score>10); anxiety was absent (score<8) in 76.76% of males and 56.12% of females, and borderline (scores between 8 and 10) in 11.97% of males and 20.86% of females. Eighteen males (12.68%) and 17 females (12.23%) resulted probably-cases of depression (HADS-Depression score>10); depression was absent (score<8) in 70.42% of males and 64.75% of females, and borderline (scores between 8 and 10) in 16.90% of males and 23.02% of females. STAXI-2 scale and subscale descriptive statistics, separately for categories of HADS-anxiety (absent, borderline, cases) and gender are shown in Table 3. Independently of gender, the anger subscale scores increased with levels of anxiety, excepted for both the Anger Control subscales with the exception for males for which the Anger Control Out scores decreased from absence to cases of anxiety. A similar pattern of associations (shown in Table 4) emerged respect to categories of HADS-depression (absent, borderline, cases). No relationship has been found between levels of Depression and Anger Control Out. The higher anger scores resulted in Anger Control In, Reaction and Anger Expression In compared to the other STAXI-2 subscale scores. STAXI-2 scale and subscale descriptive statistics, separately for Quality of Life levels (low, medium, high) and gender are shown in Table 5. For males, a low QoL score was associated with high anger subscale scores, excepted for Anger Control subscales (high scores are associated with high QoL). Among females, the relationship between anger and QoL seems to be weak, excepted for the Anger Control subscales, which elevated scores are associated with good QoL. No differences emerged between STAXI-2 scales and subscales and progression of disease (PD) among females. Males in PD feel annoyance, irritation, anger or fury (p=0.051). The predisposition to become angry is not different between those with PD and no-PD. Differently, anger experienced quickly and with little provocation was higher among patients with PD than no-PD (see Table 6).
Table 3: Descriptive statistics of STAXI-2 scales and subscales, separately for categories of HADS-Anxiety and Gender
HADS-Anxiety |
absent (<=7) |
borderline (8–10) |
cases (>=11) |
pvalue |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
4,38 |
8,07 |
10,59 |
13,30 |
28,47 |
26,66 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–4,44 |
6,67 |
4,44–13,33 |
17,78 |
6,67–53,33 |
<0,001 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
7,77 |
11,95 |
18,82 |
14,76 |
36,67 |
27,65 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
20,00 |
13,33–26,67 |
30,00 |
13,33–63,33 |
<0,001 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
3,79 |
10,48 |
8,63 |
22,58 |
32,08 |
33,40 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
16,67 |
3,33–60 |
<0,001 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
1,59 |
5,73 |
4,31 |
14,52 |
16,67 |
27,76 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,00 |
0–23,33 |
0,002 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
21,37 |
17,80 |
33,33 |
25,32 |
39,81 |
23,17 |
|
median, IQR |
14,81 |
7,41–29,63 |
25,93 |
14,81–44,44 |
38,89 |
20,37–53,70 |
0,002 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
15,60 |
18,95 |
29,90 |
25,01 |
31,25 |
22,87 |
|
median, IQR |
8,33 |
0–25 |
25,00 |
8,33–50 |
33,33 |
8,33–41,67 |
0,002 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
28,44 |
22,60 |
38,24 |
29,76 |
48,44 |
29,85 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–41,67 |
25,00 |
25–33,33 |
37,50 |
29,17–79,17 |
0,017 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
22,13 |
14,86 |
25,74 |
16,35 |
33,85 |
18,81 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
8,33–33,33 |
25,00 |
12,5–33,33 |
31,25 |
22,92–43,75 |
0,038 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
31,15 |
18,73 |
42,89 |
17,85 |
46,88 |
18,35 |
|
median, IQR |
29,17 |
16,67–41,67 |
45,83 |
29,17–54,17 |
45,83 |
35,42–62,5 |
0,001 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
69,46 |
24,68 |
60,50 |
19,47 |
51,19 |
28,70 |
|
median, IQR |
76,19 |
57,14–90,48 |
57,14 |
42,86–71,43 |
50,00 |
30,95–73,81 |
0,016 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
59,82 |
24,03 |
58,82 |
17,61 |
49,22 |
26,93 |
|
median, IQR |
62,50 |
45,83–79,17 |
54,17 |
50–66,67 |
43,75 |
33,33–72,92 |
0,289 |
HADS-Anxiety |
absent (<=7) |
borderline (8–10) |
cases (>=11) |
pvalue |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
5,36 |
8,55 |
11,57 |
15,53 |
25,56 |
22,01 |
|
median, IQR |
2,22 |
0–6,67 |
4,44 |
2,22–20 |
18,89 |
8,89–43,33 |
<0,001 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
9,40 |
14,71 |
16,78 |
15,80 |
36,25 |
25,34 |
|
median, IQR |
6,67 |
0–13,33 |
13,33 |
6,67–20 |
36,67 |
10–56,67 |
<0,001 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
5,56 |
12,73 |
12,41 |
21,58 |
27,71 |
30,06 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,00 |
0–20 |
20,00 |
0–46,67 |
<0,001 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
1,11 |
3,47 |
5,52 |
14,04 |
12,71 |
19,63 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0 |
0,00 |
0–0 |
0,00 |
0–20 |
<0,001 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
22,27 |
16,16 |
32,57 |
23,31 |
43,63 |
18,69 |
|
median, IQR |
18,52 |
11,11–33,33 |
29,63 |
14,81–48,15 |
44,44 |
29,63–55,55 |
<0,001 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
17,95 |
15,90 |
28,16 |
20,94 |
39,32 |
21,30 |
|
median, IQR |
16,67 |
8,33–25 |
25,00 |
8,33–41,67 |
33,33 |
25–54,17 |
<0,001 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
27,99 |
21,36 |
38,51 |
27,59 |
47,48 |
21,58 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–41,67 |
33,33 |
16,67–58,33 |
50,00 |
33,33–62,5 |
<0,001 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
21,63 |
14,50 |
25,86 |
14,23 |
31,77 |
19,10 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
12,5–29,17 |
25,00 |
16,67–37,5 |
27,08 |
20,83–39,58 |
0,016 |
|
Expression In |
mean, SD |
28,79 |
19,83 |
30,89 |
16,20 |
48,18 |
23,45 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
12,5–45,83 |
29,17 |
20,83–41,67 |
47,92 |
29,17–60,42 |
<0,001 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
62,82 |
25,87 |
55,67 |
23,64 |
57,89 |
25,11 |
|
median, IQR |
66,67 |
42,86–85,71 |
52,38 |
38,09–76,19 |
57,14 |
40,48–80,95 |
0,280 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
59,56 |
24,82 |
52,30 |
21,20 |
56,12 |
24,20 |
|
median, IQR |
62,50 |
41,67–75 |
54,17 |
37,5–66,67 |
62,50 |
33,33–75 |
0,211 |
* Kruskal-wallis test
Table 4: Descriptive statistics of STAXI-2 scales and subscales, separately for categories of HADS-Depression and gender
HADS, Depression |
absent (<=7) |
borderline (8–10) |
cases (>=11) |
p-value |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
4,09 |
8,10 |
15,28 |
21,48 |
18,77 |
19,97 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–4,44 |
6,67 |
3,33–18,89 |
10,00 |
6,67–31,11 |
<0,001 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
6,60 |
9,11 |
23,61 |
23,91 |
29,60 |
24,21 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
20,00 |
6,67–30,00 |
23,33 |
13,33–40 |
<0,001 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
3,93 |
12,72 |
14,17 |
26,25 |
18,89 |
25,90 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
6,67 |
0–33,33 |
<0,001 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
1,73 |
7,62 |
8,06 |
19,75 |
8,15 |
18,37 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,006 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
21,19 |
18,52 |
31,64 |
18,85 |
36,42 |
26,17 |
|
median, IQR |
14,81 |
7,41–29,63 |
29,63 |
22,22–50,00 |
24,07 |
18,52–62,96 |
0,003 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
15,25 |
19,21 |
27,78 |
21,80 |
28,70 |
24,12 |
|
median, IQR |
8,33 |
0–25,00 |
33,33 |
4,17–41,67 |
20,83 |
8,33–50,00 |
0,003 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
28,33 |
23,21 |
36,46 |
25,63 |
45,37 |
30,28 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–33,33 |
33,33 |
12,5–54,17 |
37,50 |
25–83,33 |
0.024 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
21,88 |
14,57 |
28,65 |
16,95 |
28,70 |
19,43 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
8,33–31,25 |
27,08 |
16,67–41,67 |
27,08 |
16,67–41,67 |
0,105 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
30,83 |
19,27 |
41,32 |
17,55 |
44,44 |
16,85 |
|
median, IQR |
27,08 |
16,67–41,67 |
41,67 |
29,17–54,17 |
45,83 |
33,33–54,17 |
0,002 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
70,90 |
24,48 |
59,92 |
20,30 |
49.47 |
27,14 |
|
median, IQR |
76,19 |
57,14–90,48 |
59,52 |
42,86–76,19 |
45,24 |
33,33–71,43 |
0,001 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
62,33 |
23,42 |
52,78 |
21,02 |
44,91 |
23,94 |
|
median, IQR |
66,67 |
47,92–79,17 |
54,17 |
43,75–66,67 |
43,75 |
29,17–58,33 |
0,004 |
HADS, Depression |
absent (<=7) |
borderline (8–10) |
cases (>=11) |
p-value |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
7,41 |
12,64 |
17,78 |
20,59 |
19,74 |
18,34 |
|
median, IQR |
2,22 |
0–8,89 |
13,33 |
0–22,22 |
17,78 |
2,22–24,44 |
<0.001 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
12,22 |
17,20 |
23,75 |
23,34 |
30,59 |
25,06 |
|
median, IQR |
6,67 |
0–13,33 |
20,00 |
0–43,33 |
33,33 |
6,67–53,33 |
0.002 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
7,48 |
17,04 |
19,58 |
27,09 |
22,35 |
25,71 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
10,00 |
0–26,67 |
13,33 |
0–33,33 |
0.001 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
2,52 |
8,75 |
10,00 |
17.92 |
6,27 |
14,43 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–16,67 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,038 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
26,01 |
18,61 |
31,60 |
23,48 |
42,70 |
17,23 |
|
median, IQR |
22,22 |
11,11–37,04 |
29,62 |
12,96–44,44 |
40,74 |
33,33–55,56 |
0,004 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
21,85 |
17,47 |
26,56 |
23,99 |
38,72 |
21,44 |
|
median, IQR |
16,67 |
8,33–33,33 |
25,00 |
8,33–41,67 |
33,33 |
25–58,33 |
0,012 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
31,57 |
23,62 |
37,05 |
25,49 |
49,51 |
21,14 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–50,00 |
33,33 |
16,67–58,33 |
50,00 |
33,33–66,67 |
0,013 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
22,96 |
14,54 |
26,95 |
17,13 |
30,88 |
20,20 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
12,5–33,33 |
22,92 |
14,58–37,50 |
25,00 |
20,83–37,50 |
0,227 |
|
Expression In |
mean, SD |
30,60 |
20,68 |
32,29 |
16,93 |
52,70 |
24,24 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
16,67–45,83 |
31,25 |
20,83–41,67 |
50,00 |
33,33–66,67 |
0,003 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
63,76 |
23,89 |
52,83 |
26,43 |
55,18 |
27,72 |
|
median, IQR |
66,67 |
47,92–80,95 |
50,00 |
33,33–76,19 |
61,94 |
38,10–76,19 |
0,083 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
60,93 |
23,50 |
50,39 |
23,00 |
50,74 |
25,44 |
|
median, IQR |
64,58 |
45,83–75,00 |
45,83 |
35,42–66,67 |
54,17 |
29,17–75,00 |
0,029 |
* Kruskal-wallis test
Table 5: Descriptive statistics of STAXI-2 scales and subscales, separately for Quality of Life and gender
Quality of Life |
low (1–3) |
medium (4–7) |
high (8–10) |
pvalue |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
21,11 |
24,72 |
8,01 |
13,27 |
5,09 |
11,21 |
|
median, IQR |
7,78 |
2,22–41,11 |
4,44 |
0–8,89 |
0,00 |
0–4,44 |
0,002 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
30,56 |
30,01 |
13,73 |
16,53 |
7,31 |
12,11 |
|
median, IQR |
16,67 |
6,67–56,67 |
6,67 |
0–20,00 |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
0,001 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
25,00 |
33,77 |
6,27 |
14,66 |
5,59 |
16,69 |
|
median, IQR |
6,67 |
0–50,00 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,017 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
7,78 |
14,17 |
4,02 |
13,97 |
2,37 |
9,96 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–10,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,040 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
41,36 |
23,79 |
26,31 |
20,99 |
20,13 |
17,22 |
|
median, IQR |
44,44 |
24,07–55,56 |
20,37 |
11,11–37,04 |
14,81 |
7,41–25,93 |
0,006 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
34,72 |
22,43 |
20,96 |
21,57 |
13,98 |
18,47 |
|
median, IQR |
33,33 |
12,5–54,17 |
16,67 |
0–33,33 |
8,33 |
0–25,00 |
0,004 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
46,53 |
27,40 |
33,46 |
26,33 |
27,28 |
22,29 |
|
median, IQR |
41,67 |
29,17–62,50 |
25,00 |
16,67–45,83 |
25,00 |
8,33–33-33 |
0,032 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
31,60 |
22,29 |
24,94 |
14,32 |
21,24 |
15,68 |
|
median, IQR |
29,17 |
14,58–39,58 |
20,83 |
14,58–33,33 |
16,67 |
8,33–29,17 |
0,131 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
38,54 |
19,15 |
39,52 |
18,37 |
27,82 |
18,81 |
|
median, IQR |
35,42 |
29,17–47,92 |
41,67 |
25–52,08 |
25,00 |
8,33–41,67 |
0,001 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
40,87 |
19,63 |
70,03 |
22,77 |
67,20 |
26,17 |
|
median, IQR |
38,10 |
28,57–52,38 |
73,81 |
52,38–90,48 |
71,43 |
52,38–85,71 |
0,001 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
36,46 |
15,19 |
60,23 |
21,48 |
60,89 |
25,59 |
|
median, IQR |
41,67 |
29,17–45,83 |
60,42 |
45,83–77,08 |
66,67 |
45,83–79,17 |
0,001 |
Quality of Life |
low (1–3) |
medium (4–7) |
high (8–10) |
pvalue |
||||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
12,41 |
10,05 |
13,33 |
17,38 |
7,91 |
15,44 |
|
median, IQR |
15,56 |
2,22–17,78 |
6,67 |
2,22–20,00 |
2,22 |
0–6,67 |
0,008 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
25,56 |
20,66 |
19,05 |
20,34 |
12,13 |
20,90 |
|
median, IQR |
20 |
6,67–46,67 |
13,33 |
0–26,67 |
6,67 |
0–13,33 |
0,006 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
8,89 |
11,84 |
15,57 |
24,49 |
7,33 |
17,68 |
|
median, IQR |
0 |
0–20,00 |
0 |
0–20,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,023 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
2,78 |
6,64 |
5,28 |
13,86 |
4,27 |
11,33 |
|
median, IQR |
0 |
0–0,00 |
0 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,868 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
34,26 |
23,64 |
32,28 |
21,62 |
23,63 |
16,05 |
|
median, IQR |
27,78 |
16,67–55,56 |
29,63 |
14,81–44,44 |
22,22 |
11,11–33,33 |
0,078 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
31,94 |
25,08 |
27,16 |
21,90 |
20,00 |
14,96 |
|
median, IQR |
25 |
16,67–50,00 |
25 |
8,33–41,67 |
16,67 |
8,33–33,33 |
0,194 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
36,81 |
29,4 |
38,53 |
25,22 |
29,50 |
20,91 |
|
median, IQR |
29,17 |
12,5–66,67 |
33,33 |
16,67–58,33 |
25,00 |
16,67–50,00 |
0,181 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
26,74 |
24,58 |
25,38 |
14,77 |
23,58 |
15,76 |
|
median, IQR |
18,75 |
10,42–35,42 |
25 |
12,5–33,33 |
20,83 |
12,5–33,33 |
0,618 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
30,9 |
21,35 |
37,12 |
19,78 |
29,08 |
23,34 |
|
median, IQR |
27.08 |
18,75–37,50 |
33,33 |
20,83–50,00 |
25,00 |
12,5–41,67 |
0,037 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
44,84 |
28,31 |
62,77 |
23,62 |
59,90 |
26,14 |
|
median, IQR |
42,86 |
28,57–64,29 |
66,67 |
42,86–80,95 |
61,90 |
38,09–80,95 |
0,102 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
37,15 |
23,13 |
59,63 |
22,76 |
58,42 |
24,22 |
|
median, IQR |
33,33 |
18.75–52,08 |
62,50 |
41,67–75,00 |
62,50 |
41,67–75,00 |
0,017 |
* Kruskal-wallis test
Table 6: Descriptive statistics of STAXI-2 scales and subscales by progression of disease and gender
Progression of disease |
No |
Yes |
p-value |
|||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
4,21 |
7,61 |
8,73 |
15,38 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–5,56 |
4,44 |
0–8,89 |
0.079 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
7,14 |
11,61 |
13,63 |
18,40 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
6,67 |
0–20,00 |
0,051 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
5,00 |
12,65 |
8,19 |
19,59 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–6,67 |
0,382 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
0,48 |
1,75 |
4,39 |
13,71 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,125 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
20,11 |
19,74 |
26.06 |
20,46 |
|
median, IQR |
14,81 |
5,56–25,93 |
22,22 |
11,11–37,04 |
0,120 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
12,80 |
20,22 |
20,61 |
21,01 |
|
median, IQR |
8,33 |
0–12,50 |
16,67 |
0–33,33 |
0,032 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
27,68 |
22,46 |
32,89 |
25,74 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–33,33 |
25,00 |
16,67–41,67 |
0,442 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
23,21 |
14,80 |
24,05 |
16,15 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
12,5–33,33 |
22,92 |
12,5–33,33 |
0,918 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
33,18 |
20,27 |
34,61 |
19,23 |
|
median, IQR |
31,25 |
18,75–45,83 |
33,33 |
20,83–45,83 |
0,721 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
73,13 |
19,58 |
64,66 |
26,20 |
|
median, IQR |
73,81 |
59,52–90,48 |
71,43 |
42,86–85,71 |
0,183 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
65,33 |
20,26 |
56,83 |
24,37 |
|
median, IQR |
64,58 |
52,08–83,33 |
58,33 |
41,67–75,00 |
0,113 |
Progression of disease |
No |
Yes |
p-value |
|||
Anger, state |
mean, SD |
9,98 |
12,69 |
11,94 |
17,80 |
|
median, IQR |
6,67 |
2,22–13,33 |
2,22 |
0–17,78 |
0,530 |
|
feeling |
mean, SD |
18,07 |
19,63 |
16,67 |
21,48 |
|
median, IQR |
13,33 |
6,67–20,00 |
6,67 |
0–26,67 |
0,271 |
|
verbal anger |
mean, SD |
8,44 |
16,66 |
13,83 |
23,55 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–13,33 |
0,00 |
0–20,00 |
0,261 |
|
physical anger |
mean, SD |
3,41 |
9,06 |
5,32 |
13,81 |
|
median, IQR |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,00 |
0–0,00 |
0,984 |
|
Anger, trait |
mean, SD |
29,79 |
14,71 |
29,12 |
22,56 |
|
median, IQR |
29,63 |
18,52–40,74 |
22,22 |
11,11–44,44 |
0,362 |
|
temperament |
mean, SD |
23,33 |
15,65 |
25,80 |
22,13 |
|
median, IQR |
25,00 |
8,33–33,33 |
25,00 |
8,33–33,33 |
0,875 |
|
reaction |
mean, SD |
37,41 |
19,75 |
34,04 |
26,29 |
|
median, IQR |
33,33 |
25–50,00 |
29,17 |
8,33–58,33 |
0,227 |
|
Expression Out |
mean, SD |
24,81 |
12,96 |
24,87 |
17,40 |
|
median, IQR |
20,83 |
16,67–33,33 |
20,83 |
12,5–33,33 |
0,567 |
|
Expression IN |
mean, SD |
33,89 |
18,65 |
33,60 |
22,76 |
|
median, IQR |
33,33 |
20,83–45,83 |
29,17 |
16,67–50,00 |
0,650 |
|
Control Out |
mean, SD |
60,11 |
23,31 |
60,23 |
26,26 |
|
median, IQR |
61,90 |
42,86–76,19 |
61,90 |
42,86–80,95 |
0,869 |
|
Control In |
mean, SD |
57,41 |
22,27 |
57,18 |
24,87 |
|
median, IQR |
62,50 |
41,67–75,00 |
62,50 |
41,67–75,00 |
0,941 |
* Kruskal-wallis test
Discussion
Among patients with a diagnosis of cancer, anger is one of the “expected” emotional reactions. Surprisingly we failed to found relationships between the characteristics of anger, its expression and control, and the clinical objective characteristics of the cancer as type, stadium, progression of disease, years from diagnosis, and being in chemotherapeutic treatment. Neither association have been found with patients’ sociodemographic characteristics, with the exclusion of years of education. To have less than 14 years of school is associated with high scores in State and Trait Anger, while having more than 13 years of school is associated with more Anger Control In, males and females attempted to relax them to reduce angry. In this sample we have found low scores related to the intensity of the individual’s angry feelings (Anger State), especially among males, and more high scores in the tendency to become angry when people faced with a negative or difficult to control situation (Anger Reaction). At the same time, the Anger Expression scores, especially “In”, reflect that these patients tend to suppress, repress, deny anger feelings. Together with high Anger Control scores reflecting the expenditure of energy that people do to monitoring and control the expression of angry. Generally, people emotionally over-reactive with a high control of emotions experienced an internal conflict and become anxious and depressed especially if the situation causing anger persists. In this sample, males and females showed differently their emotional state of anger. Males tend to repress anger and show a lack of control of anger towards the outside, more than females. Adopting anger repression as the preferred mode of expression of anger could lead to depression and directly affects the immune system.
Nonetheless responses of anxiety, depression and anger seem to be organized into a pattern of a general emotional reaction. A first hypothesis is that worsening mood is associated with increased communication between the amygdala and hippocampus, which are linked to emotion and memory respectively. Probably due to individual and gender differences. Testosterone has been associated with increased emotional reactivity in the brain [18] and patient’s personal history could explain why an anger mood (in the amygdala) became a trigger for the recollection of sad memories (in memory). In this framework patient’s ability to control anger could be the defense that consent patient to feel better. And we have seen that this ability differs in males and females. From an immunological point of view, we know that there are different interconnections between emotions and immune response and between gender and immune response. Gender differences have also been reported recently in the response to immunotherapy treatments by Botticelli [19]. Many genes involved in the immune response are located on the X chromosome and gender and diet are implicated in differences in the function of microbiota in the modulate of the immune system. The study of specific differences between the sexes in the processing and reactions to emotions could have important implications for immunotherapy treatment use in cancer due to its different efficacy in males and females.
From an oncological point of view some serious considerations are to be taken on commitment at this time that immunotherapy is becoming an important cancer treatment. If we want to boost the body’s natural defenses to fight cancer therefore the healthy immune systems became our primary concern. In conclusion, staff working in oncological day hospital should be trained to identify patients with emotional difficulties and facilitate referral for treatment. Today there is a growing interest in the use of patient-reported outcome measures that could capture some peculiar aspects that are not normally understood during the doctor-patient visit. Clinicians could gain important insights about their patients by looking at the result of validated self-report patient questionnaires, to identify patients with inadequate expression of emotion or too high levels of emotional reaction in order to improve quality of care and a better response to treatment.
Abbreviations: CI, Confidence Interval; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; IQR, interquartile range; PD, progression of disease; QoL, quality of life; SD, standard deviation; STAXI, State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory.
References
- Heany SJ, van Honk J, Stein DJ, Brooks SJ (2016) A quantitative and qualitative review of the effect of testosterone on the function and structure of the human social-emotional brain. Metabolic Brain Disease 31: 157–167. [Crossref]
- Van Wingen GA, Ossewaarde L, Backstrom T, Hermans EJ, Fernandez G (2011) Gonadal hormone regulation of the emotion circuity in humans. Neuroscience 191: 38–45. [Crossref]
- Barinkova K, Mesarosova M (2013) Anger, coping and quality of life in female cancer patients. Social behavior and personality 41: 135–142.
- Cox T, Mackay C (1982) Psychosocial factors and psychophysiological mechanism in the aetiology and development of cancer. Social Science & Medicine 16: 381–396.
- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, DSM-5. (2013) American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA.
- Aghaei M, Ghorbani N, Rostami R, Mahdavi A (2015) Comparison of anger management, anxiety and perceived stress in patients with cancer and Coronary Heart Disease (CHD). Journal of Medicine and Life 8: 97–101. [Crossref]
- Thomas SP, Groer M, Davis M, Droppleman P, Mozingo J, et al. (2000) Anger and cancer: an analysis of the linkages. Cancer Nurs 23: 344–349. [Crossref]
- Harburg E, Julius M, Kaciroti N, Gleiberman L, Schork MA (2003) Expressive/suppressive anger- coping responses, gender, and types of mortality: a 17-year follow-up (Tecumseh, Michigan, 1971–1988). Psychosom Med 65: 588–597.
- Appel MA, Holroyd KA, Gorkin L (2005) Anger and the etiology and progression of physical illness. In: Temoshok L, Van Dyke C, Zegans LS (eds.) Emotions in health and illness. Grune & Straton, New York, 73–87.
- Greer S, Morris T (1975) Psychological attributes of women who develop breast cancer: A controlled study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 19: 147–153.
- White VM, English DR, Coates H, Lagerlund M, Borland R, et al. (2007) Is cancer risk associated with anger control and negative affect? Findings from a prospective cohort study. Psychosom Med 69: 667–674. [Crossref]
- Berkowitz L, Harmon-Jones E (2004) Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion 4: 107–130. [Crossref]
- Spielberger CD. STAXI-2: State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2: Professional Manual. PAR Psychological Assessment Resources 1999.
- Vagg PR, Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory™ Interpretive Report (STAXI-2: IR™). PAR Psychological Assessment Resources 1999.
- Zigmond AS, Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 67: 361–370. [Crossref]
- Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D (2002) The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale: an updated literature review. J Psychosom Res 52: 69–77. [Crossref]
- Mazzotti E. Sebastiani C, Marchetti P (2012) Patient-perception of the disease control and relationship with psychosocial variables. Cancer Management and Research 4: 335–340.
- Klein S, Kruse O, Tapia Leon I, Stalder T, Stark R, et al. (2019) Increased neural reactivity to emotional pictures in men with high hair testosterone concentrations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 14: 1009–1016.
- Botticelli A, Onesti CE, Zizzari I (2017) The sexist behaviour of immune checkpoint inhibitors in cancer therapy? Oncotarget 8: 99336–99346. [Crossref]